News location:

Canberra Today 22°/26° | Tuesday, March 19, 2024 | Digital Edition | Crossword & Sudoku

Research shows birds can eavesdrop too

Fairy-wren. Photo by Jessica McLachlan
NEW research involving biologists from ANU shows that some birds are just as skilled as humans at eavesdropping.

The team, led by Prof Robert Magrath from the ANU Research School of Biology, discovered fairy-wrens quickly learn to respond to new alarm calls just by listening closely to the familiar sounds around them.

“Theoretically they could learn with their eyes closed,” Prof Magrath says.

“We knew that fairy-wrens learn to recognise alarm calls if they repeatedly see a hawk at the same time as hearing an unfamiliar call, but we suspected they could learn just by listening to familiar alarm calls, without having to see a predator at all.”

To test the idea, the team, which included Dr Dominique Potvin and Dr Chaminda Ratnayake from ANU and Prof Andrew Radford from the University of Bristol, played back sounds to fairy-wrens in the National Botanic Gardens in Canberra.

The idea was to see if the birds could learn to identify an unfamiliar sound as an alarm call if it occurred at the same time as a chorus of familiar calls.

“We made the experiment as realistic as possible by using stereo speakers to simulate a natural chorus of alarm calls from birds common in the Gardens. But we also snuck in an unfamiliar sound, to see if they could learn about it,” Dr Potvin says.

Prior to the experiment fairy-wrens ignored the unfamiliar sounds, but after just two days’ training with the chorus most of them fled to cover after hearing the new sound – they’d learnt it was an alarm call.

The new findings show what’s called “social learning”, which is where individuals learn from others, rather than having to learn from direct experience.

“Which is a good thing, because learning about predators by direct experience could prove lethal!” Dr Potvin says.

“Social learning is important because it can lead to rapid spread of behaviour through a population. The birds learnt this new sound means ‘predator’ without having to see a predator or even see callers fleeing for cover. Our results help explain why eavesdropping is so common across species.”

The project highlights the potential impact human interference could have on a delicate environment.

“If you drive a species to extinction it means not only is that species missing from the community, but any information that it provided about danger is also missing,” Prof Magrath says.

“In the Botanic Gardens, for example, New Holland honeyeaters are very observant and have very loud alarm calls, so they’re an important source of information about hawks for other birds. If you took away that species you’d put others in greater danger.”

Professor Magrath believes the group’s findings could also help protect rare species.

“When threatened species are released into the wild after captive breeding, they often make easy pickings for predators,” he says.

“Our work suggests a way of teaching them to recognise alarm calls at the release site. Captive breeding programs are expensive, so any extra chance for survival is worth investigating.”

Who can be trusted?

In a world of spin and confusion, there’s never been a more important time to support independent journalism in Canberra.

If you trust our work online and want to enforce the power of independent voices, I invite you to make a small contribution.

Every dollar of support is invested back into our journalism to help keep citynews.com.au strong and free.

Become a supporter

Thank you,

Ian Meikle, editor

Share this

Leave a Reply

Follow us on Instagram @canberracitynews